Balanced Training vs. Evidence-Based Scientific Dog Training Methods.

balanced dog training positive reinforcement training

Balanced Training vs. Evidence-Based Scientific Dog Training Methods by Will Bangura, M.S., CBCC-KA, CPDT-KA, FFCP

First and foremost, I would like to disclose that in the past, I had practiced the “Balanced Trainer” approach, which involved utilizing aversive training techniques and tools, such as prong, choke, and electronic collars to correct dog behavior. However, as someone who has been training dogs for over 35 years, I have concluded that such methods and tools are not necessary and can, in fact, be harmful to our furry friends.

While I have experienced success in training dogs using aversive methods, I have since adopted a positive reinforcement approach, which has proven to be just as effective, if not more so, in achieving desired behavior outcomes. I firmly believe that any behavior can be trained or corrected using positive reinforcement and that no dog, regardless of its behavior or disposition, requires fear, pain, or intimidation to learn.

In this article, I will present a comparative analysis of the balanced training and positive reinforcement methods, citing relevant research studies and scientific evidence. Ultimately, it is up to you to decide which approach aligns with your values and yields the best results for your furry companion.

 Positive Reinforcement Training Methods.

Building Trust and Enhancing Relationships.

 According to Rooney and Cowan (2011), positive reinforcement training methods help to establish trust and build a bond between dogs and their pet guardians, leading to a stronger and more fulfilling relationship. These methods use rewards and praise to encourage desired behaviors, which promotes trust and connection between the dog and the handler.

Effectiveness and Cognitive Development.

Studies have demonstrated that positive reinforcement methods are more effective than punishment-based methods in promoting good behavior in dogs (Schilder and van der Borg, 2004). Dogs trained using positive reinforcement methods also show better problem-solving abilities and cognitive development compared to those trained using punishment-based methods (McGowan et al., 2013). These methods also provide mental stimulation and promote physical exercise, enhancing the overall well-being of the dog.

Long-Term Behavior Change.

Positive reinforcement methods not only promote immediate behavior change but also lead to long-term behavior change (Rooney and Cowan, 2011). By teaching dogs to associate desirable behaviors with rewards, a long-lasting change in behavior can be achieved. These methods also address the root causes of problem behaviors, which can help prevent the recurrence of these behaviors in the future.

Safety and Well-being.

Positive reinforcement methods promote safety for the dog and the people around them (Hiby et al., 2004). By focusing on teaching the dog desirable behaviors and rewarding them for exhibiting those behaviors, the risk of unwanted or aggressive behavior is reduced. In contrast, punishment-based methods may increase the risk of aggressive behavior and can cause physical harm to the dog.

Balanced Training Methods.

Traditional balanced training methods combine the use of punishment and corrections with positive reinforcement. Proponents of balanced training argue that punishment and corrections are necessary to address unwanted behaviors in dogs. According to Blackwell et al. (2008), evidence suggests that balanced training methods may not be as effective or humane as positive reinforcement methods. Punishment and aversive techniques can create fear and anxiety in dogs, leading to a breakdown in communication and decreased overall well-being.

 The Argument Against Balanced Training Methods.

The use of aversive tools such as shock collars and choke chains in punishment-based training methods can increase the risk of injury to the dog (Hiby et al., 2004). These tools can cause physical harm, including burns, bruises, and injuries to the neck and spine. Additionally, the benefits of punishment-based methods may not be long-lasting, as they often suppress undesirable behaviors temporarily without addressing the root cause of the behavior.

Training High-Drive and Easily Distracted Dogs.

 Balanced trainers often argue that positive reinforcement-only training has its limits and is not effective for training high-drive dogs or dogs that are easily distracted. Svartberg and Forkman (2002) demonstrate that high-drive dogs can be trained using positive reinforcement methods alone. The key to training high-drive dogs or dogs that are easily distracted is to tailor the training to the individual needs of the dog, gradually exposing the dog to distractions, and using positive reinforcement only.

Myths and Misconceptions.

The Limits of Positive Reinforcement.

The argument that positive reinforcement-only training has its limits is based on a lack of understanding and skill on the part of balanced trainers. With patience, consistency, and skill, positive reinforcement training can be highly effective for training dogs, including high-drive, and easily distracted dogs. The use of punishment-based methods, such as shock collars or prong collars, may cause more harm than good and does not necessarily lead to long-lasting behavior change.

The Science Behind Dog Training.

Overall, positive reinforcement training methods are evidence-based and scientifically proven to be effective, humane, and safe ways to train dogs (Arhant et al., 2010). These methods promote a positive relationship between the dog and their pet guardian, support cognitive development, and facilitate long-term behavior change. In contrast, punishment-based and balanced training methods can lead to fear, anxiety, and physical harm in dogs, with potentially short-lived benefits.

Choosing the Right Training Method.

As a dog pet guardian, it is crucial to choose training methods grounded in scientific evidence that promote the well-being of your dog. Positive reinforcement training methods are evidence-based and scientifically proven to be effective, humane, and safe ways to train dogs (Horowitz, 2009). These methods promote a positive relationship between the dog and their pet guardian, promote cognitive development, and long-term behavior change. In contrast, punishment-based training methods can cause fear, anxiety, and physical harm to dogs, and their benefits may not be long-lasting (Horowitz, 2009). Dog pet guardians should choose training methods that are based on scientific evidence and promote the well-being of their dog, and positive reinforcement training methods are an excellent choice for achieving these goals (Horowitz, 2009).

Positive Reinforcement for Specific Tasks.

Positive reinforcement training is not only effective for teaching basic obedience and solving problem behaviors, but it can also be used to train dogs for specific tasks such as search and rescue, therapy work, or agility competitions (Yin, 2015). By tailoring the training to the individual needs of the dog, considering their age, breed, and temperament, positive reinforcement methods can help dogs reach their full potential (Yin, 2015).

Debunking the Myth of High-Drive Dogs.

The myth that positive reinforcement has its limits and that punishment-based methods are necessary for high-drive dogs or dogs that are heavily distracted is just that, a myth (Arhant et al., 2010). Positive reinforcement training can be tailored to the individual needs of the dog and has been shown to be effective in training dogs for a variety of activities, including protection sports and hunting (Arhant et al., 2010).

Setting Boundaries with Positive Reinforcement.

It is important to remember that positive reinforcement training does not mean permissive training (Pryor, 2019). Dogs need clear boundaries and rules, and training should be consistent and fair. However, these boundaries can be set using positive reinforcement methods such as reward-based training and using positive cues (Pryor, 2019). By gradually exposing the dog to distractions and working within their threshold, even high-drive and highly distracted dogs can be successfully trained using positive reinforcement methods (Pryor, 2019).

The Skills Needed for Effective Positive Reinforcement.

The arguments put forth by balanced trainers that positive reinforcement-only training has its limits and that punishment-based methods are necessary for certain types of dogs are not based on fact, but rather on a lack of skill and understanding of how to effectively use positive reinforcement methods (Pryor, 2019). With patience, consistency, and skill, positive reinforcement training can be a highly effective way to train dogs of all types and temperaments (Pryor, 2019).

The Best Interests of the Dog and Pet Guardian.

In the end, the choice of training method should be based on the best interests of the dog and their pet guardian. Evidence-based and science-based dog training methods that incorporate positive reinforcement are proven to be effective, humane, and safe for both the dog and their pet guardian (Dunbar, 2018). By choosing these methods, dog pet guardians can build a strong, positive relationship with their dog, foster their cognitive development, and promote long-term behavior change, ultimately leading to a happier and healthier life for both the dog and their pet guardian (Dunbar, 2018).

Balanced Training Methods: A Comprehensive Look.

 Traditional Balanced Training: Pros and Cons.

 Balanced training methods, which involve both punishment and positive reinforcement, have been used for many years in dog training (Hiby et al., 2004). Advocates of this approach argue that operant conditioning, the process by which animals learn, involves both punishers and negative reinforcement, and that addressing unwanted behaviors in dogs requires the use of punishment and corrections (Hiby et al., 2004).

Critiques of Balanced Training.

However, an increasing body of evidence suggests that balanced training methods may not be as effective or humane as positive reinforcement methods (Horowitz, 2009). The use of punishment and aversive techniques can create fear and anxiety in dogs, leading to a breakdown in communication and a decrease in overall well-being (Horowitz, 2009). Aversive tools, such as shock collars and choke chains, can cause physical harm, including burns, bruises, and injuries to the neck and spine (Horowitz, 2009).

Positive Reinforcement Training: Benefits and Applications.

In contrast, evidence-based and science-based dog training methods that focus on positive reinforcement are effective, humane, and safe ways to train dogs (Horowitz, 2009). Positive reinforcement promotes a positive relationship between dogs and their pet guardians, providing mental stimulation and physical exercise, leading to happier and healthier dogs (Horowitz, 2009). Long-term behavior change is achieved through addressing the root causes of problem behaviors and promoting desirable behaviors (Horowitz, 2009).

Scientific Support for Positive Reinforcement.

Numerous studies demonstrate the benefits of positive reinforcement training methods and the negative consequences of punishment-based methods (Horowitz, 2009; Yin, 2015). Using positive reinforcement has been found to promote cognitive development, long-term behavior change, and a positive relationship between dogs and their pet guardians (Horowitz, 2009; Yin, 2015). It also ensures safety for the dog and the people around them (Horowitz, 2009).

Tailoring to Individual Needs.

Positive reinforcement methods can be adapted to suit the individual needs of dogs, considering their age, breed, and temperament (Yin, 2015). These training methods can be used for basic obedience, solving problem behaviors, and even training dogs for specific tasks such as search and rescue, therapy work, or agility competitions (Yin, 2015). By tailoring the training approach to the dog’s individual needs, positive reinforcement methods can help dogs reach their full potential.

Debunking Myths: Positive Reinforcement Limits and High-Drive Dogs.

Myth: Positive Reinforcement Isn’t Enough.

Balanced training advocates often argue that positive reinforcement has its limits and that some dogs, particularly high-drive dogs, or heavily distracted dogs, require corrections because positive reinforcement alone is not enough. The myth perpetuated by balanced trainers that positive reinforcement has its limits and that some dogs require corrections because positive reinforcement alone is not enough has been disproven by numerous examples (Arhant et al., 2010). Positive reinforcement training can be tailored to the individual needs of the dog, including high-drive dogs or heavily distracted dogs, and has been shown to be effective in training dogs for a variety of activities (Arhant et al., 2010). Dogs trained using positive reinforcement methods have won championships in various disciplines, including protection sports, agility, AKC Obedience trials, hunting, and herding livestock (Arhant et al., 2010).

Tailoring Training for High-Drive and Easily Distracted Dogs.

The key to training high-drive or easily distracted dogs is not relying on punishment-based methods but tailoring the training to the individual needs of the dog (Yin, 2015). Food-motivated dogs may respond well to reward-based training, while toy-motivated dogs may respond better to toy-based rewards. Gradually increasing the level of distraction during training can teach the dog to remain focused on their handler even in the presence of distractions (Yin, 2015).

Setting Boundaries with Positive Reinforcement.

Positive reinforcement training does not mean permissive training (Pryor, 2019). Dogs need clear boundaries and rules, and training should be consistent and fair. These boundaries can be set using positive reinforcement methods such as reward-based training and positive cues (Pryor, 2019).

Refuting Balanced Training Arguments.

Balanced Training Argument: Necessity of Corrections.

The balanced trainers’ claim that without corrections or telling the dog “No,” training is ineffective is not based on fact but rather on their lack of skill and understanding of positive reinforcement training for high-drive and easily distracted dogs (Pryor, 2019). Many positive reinforcement trainers have successfully trained high-drive and easily distracted dogs (Pryor, 2019). The problem lies in the balanced trainers’ lack of skill and understanding of positive reinforcement training for these types of dogs, rather than the method itself (Pryor, 2019).

Gradual Exposure and Thresholds.

Gradual exposure to distractions is a crucial part of positive reinforcement training (McGowan et al., 2013). By starting with low-level distractions and gradually increasing the level of difficulty, the dog can learn to remain focused on their handler and ignore distractions (McGowan et al., 2013). The key is to keep the dog below their threshold so that they do not become overwhelmed, stressed, or anxious (McGowan et al., 2013).

Positive reinforcement training does not mean that the dog is never told “no” or given feedback (McGowan et al., 2013). It simply means that feedback is given in a positive and constructive way that promotes good behavior rather than punishing bad behavior. Positive reinforcement trainers use a variety of techniques such as redirecting the dog’s attention, withholding rewards for undesirable behavior, and using positive cues to communicate with the dog (McGowan et al., 2013)

Debunking the “Gentle Tactile Cue” Myth.

The argument put forth by balanced trainers that the use of a shock collar or a prong collar is just a tactile cue to get the dog’s attention and is not painful is a myth perpetuated by balanced trainers to justify the use of aversive techniques (Hiby et al., 2004). The truth is that the use of a shock collar or a prong collar can cause significant pain, fear, and anxiety for the dog (Hiby et al., 2004).

Positive reinforcement trainers use a variety of techniques such as tapping the dog’s shoulder or using a verbal cue to get the dog’s attention, which are effective and humane and do not cause pain or fear in the dog (Hiby et al., 2004).

The argument put forth by balanced trainers that they need to use aversive techniques to train high-drive dogs or highly distracted dogs is not based on fact but on a lack of skill and education in positive reinforcement training methods (Rooney & Cowan, 2011). Positive reinforcement training can be effective for training all types of dogs, including high-drive dogs and highly distracted dogs (Rooney & Cowan, 2011).

The key to training high-drive dogs or highly distracted dogs is to gradually expose the dog to distractions and use positive reinforcement methods to teach the dog to focus on their handler (Rooney & Cowan, 2011). This process takes time, patience, and skill, but it is an effective and humane way to train dogs.

Positive reinforcement training methods are evidence-based and scientifically proven to be effective, humane, and safe ways to train dogs (Rooney & Cowan, 2011). These methods promote a positive relationship between the dog and their pet guardian, promote cognitive development, and long-term behavior change. In contrast, punishment-based training methods can cause fear, anxiety, and physical harm to dogs, and their benefits may not be long-lasting. Dog pet guardians should choose training methods that are based on scientific evidence and promote the well-being of their dog, and positive reinforcement training methods are an excellent choice for achieving these goals.

Choosing the Right Training Method.

 When selecting a training method for your dog, it is important to consider their individual needs, breed, age, and temperament (Reid, 2009). Research various training methods and consult with professional trainers or behaviorists who practice positive reinforcement techniques (Reid, 2009). Attending workshops, seminars, and conferences on dog training and behavior can also help deepen your understanding of the principles of positive reinforcement training (Reid, 2009).

The Importance of Consistency and Patience.

 Regardless of the training method you choose, consistency and patience are key components of successful dog training (Hiby et al., 2004). Set clear rules and boundaries for your dog and ensure that everyone in the household follows these guidelines (Hiby et al., 2004). Keep training sessions short and engaging, and gradually increase the level of difficulty as your dog progresses. Remember that each dog is unique, and it may take some time for them to learn new behaviors or break old habits (Hiby et al., 2004).

The Benefits of Positive Reinforcement Training for Dog Pet Guardians.

 Not only is positive reinforcement training beneficial for dogs, but it also offers numerous advantages for dog pet guardians (Rooney & Cowan, 2011). By using positive reinforcement methods, you will build a strong, trust-based relationship with your dog, making training sessions more enjoyable and effective (Rooney & Cowan, 2011). Additionally, positive reinforcement training often leads to increased confidence in handling your dog and a deeper understanding of canine behavior (Rooney & Cowan, 2011).

 Positive reinforcement training methods are proven to be effective, humane, and safe for dogs, promoting a positive relationship between the dog and their pet guardian, supporting cognitive development, and facilitating long-term behavior change (Rooney & Cowan, 2011). In contrast, punishment-based training methods can lead to fear, anxiety, and physical harm in dogs, with potentially short-lived benefits (Rooney & Cowan, 2011).

As a dog pet guardian, it is crucial to choose training methods grounded in scientific evidence that promote the well-being of your dog (Reid, 2009). Positive reinforcement training methods, when tailored to your dog’s individual needs and coupled with consistency and patience, are the ideal choice for achieving these goals (Hiby et al., 2004). By embracing positive reinforcement training, you are setting your dog up for success and laying the foundation for a happy, healthy, and harmonious life together (Rooney & Cowan, 2011).

 Organizations Condemning Aversive Training Methods.

Here are some of the organizations that have issued position statements or public statements condemning the use of aversive training methods for dogs:

  • American Veterinary Society of Animal Behavior (AVSAB)
  • American Animal Hospital Association (AAHA)
  • Association of Professional Dog Trainers (APDT)
  • International Association of Animal Behavior Consultants (IAABC)
  • Pet Professional Guild (PPG)
  • British Veterinary Association (BVA)
  • British Small Animal Veterinary Association (BSAVA)
  • Royal Society for the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals (RSPCA)
  • The Kennel Club (UK)
  • The Canadian Veterinary Medical Association (CVMA)
  • The Canadian Association of Professional Pet Dog Trainers (CAPPDT)
  • The Australian Veterinary Association (AVA)
  • The Australian Association of Professional Dog Trainers (AAPDT)

These organizations recognize the potential harm that aversive training methods can cause to dogs, as well as the effectiveness and safety of positive reinforcement-based training methods.

  • The American Humane Society
  • The Humane Society of the United States (HSUS)
  • The American Society for the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals (ASPCA)
  • The National Association of Dog Obedience Instructors (NADOI)
  • The Pet Industry Joint Advisory Council (PIJAC)
  • The Certification Council for Professional Dog Trainers (CCPDT)
  • The International Association of Animal Behavior Consultants.

Here are some countries, provinces, territories, states, and cities that have banned or restricted the use of certain aversive training tools for dogs.

  • Scotland: banned the use of shock collars in 2018
  • Wales: banned the use of shock collars in 2010
  • British Columbia, Canada: banned the use of shock collars in 2010
  • Quebec, Canada: banned the use of shock collars in 2019
  • Germany: banned the use of shock collars in 2019
  • Denmark: banned the use of shock collars in 2018
  • Norway: banned the use of shock collars in 1988
  • Sweden: banned the use of shock collars in 2021
  • Finland: banned the use of shock collars in 2019
  • Austria: banned the use of shock collars in 2019
  • New Zealand: banned the use of electronic collars and prong collars in 2020
  • Australian Capital Territory, Australia: banned the use of shock collars, prong collars, and choke collars in 2019
  • South Australia, Australia: banned the use of shock collars in 2018
  • Quebec City, Canada: banned the use of prong and choke collars in 2021

What the Peer Reviewed Scientific Studies Have to Say.

These studies, along with many others, provide evidence that the use of aversive methods and tools in dog training is unnecessary and can cause more harm than good. Positive reinforcement training methods are effective, humane, and promote a strong bond between the dog and their pet guardian.   

1. Cooper, J. J., Cracknell, N., Hardiman, J., Wright, H., & Mills, D. (2014)

Summary of findings: This study compared the welfare consequences and efficacy of electronic training collars to reward-based training methods in pet dogs. The researchers found that dogs trained with electronic collars exhibited more signs of stress, such as elevated cortisol levels and increased vocalizations, compared to dogs trained using positive reinforcement techniques. Additionally, the study found that electronic collar training was less effective in teaching the dogs to perform new tasks compared to positive reinforcement training. The researchers concluded that the use of electronic collars in dog training may have negative welfare consequences and is less effective than reward-based methods.

2. Blackwell, E. J., Bolster, C., Richards, G., Loftus, B. A., & Casey, R. A. (2012)

Summary of findings: This study investigated the prevalence, reasons for use, risk factors, and pet guardian-perceived success of electronic collar training compared to other training methods. According to the results, 3.3% of the surveyed dog pet guardians reported using electronic collars. Electronic collar use was more common among pet guardians who had larger dogs, dogs with existing behavior problems, and those who used other aversive training methods. The study found that the pet guardian-perceived success of electronic collar training was lower than for reward-based training methods. The researchers concluded that electronic collar use is relatively uncommon and is associated with specific pet guardian, dog, and training factors, with reward-based training being perceived as more successful overall.

3. Schalke, E., Stichnoth, J., Ott, S., & Jones-Baade, R. (2007)

Summary of findings: This study examined the clinical signs and potential adverse effects of electronic collar training on dogs in everyday situations. The researchers found that the use of electronic collars led to stress reactions in dogs, including elevated cortisol levels, increased heart rate, and increased stress-related behaviors. These stress reactions were observed even when the electronic collars were used according to the manufacturer’s guidelines. The study concluded that electronic collar training may have negative welfare implications for dogs and suggested that alternative training methods should be considered.

4. Arhant, C., Bubna-Littitz, H., Bartels, A., Futschik, A., & Troxler, J. (2010)

Summary of findings: This study investigated the effects of different training methods, pet guardian behavior consistency, and engagement in activities on the behavior of smaller and larger dogs. The researchers found that the use of aversive training methods, such as physical punishment and negative reinforcement, was associated with more problematic behaviors in dogs. On the other hand, the use of positive reinforcement and reward-based training methods resulted in fewer behavior problems. Additionally, the study found that inconsistencies in pet guardian behavior and a low level of engagement in activities with the dog were linked to more problematic behaviors in both smaller and larger dogs. The researchers concluded that the use of reward-based training methods and consistent pet guardian behavior, as well as engaging in activities with the dog, are essential for promoting positive canine behavior.

5. Blackwell, E. J., Twells, C. L., Seawright, A., & Casey, R. A. (2008)

Summary of findings: This study examined the relationship between dog training methods and the occurrence of behavior problems as reported by dog pet guardians. The researchers found that the use of punishment-based training methods was associated with an increased risk of behavior problems in dogs, particularly aggression and fear-related behaviors. In contrast, the use of reward-based training methods was linked to a lower occurrence of behavior problems. The study also found that dogs trained using a mix of positive reinforcement and punishment techniques exhibited a higher rate of behavior problems compared to those trained exclusively with positive reinforcement methods. The researchers concluded that the use of punishment-based training methods may contribute to the development of behavior problems in dogs and that positive reinforcement training is a more effective and safer alternative.

6. Miklósi, Á., Kubinyi, E., Topál, J., Gácsi, M., Virányi, Zs., & Csányi, V. (2003)

Summary of findings: This study examined the differences in the ability of wolves and dogs to follow human gaze and engage in communicative interactions. While not directly related to training methods, the findings of this study provide insights into the unique relationship between humans and dogs that has evolved over time. The researchers found that dogs were more likely to follow human gaze and look back at humans compared to wolves, even when both species were raised under similar conditions. This suggests that dogs have evolved specific skills that facilitate communication and cooperation with humans. These skills could potentially contribute to the success of positive reinforcement training methods that rely on social interactions and communication between the dog and its handler.

7. Ziv, G. (2017) Ziv, G. (2017). The effects of using aversive training methods in dogs-A review. Journal of Veterinary Behavior, 19, 50-60.

Summary of findings: This review paper examined the effects of using aversive training methods, including electronic collars, choke chains, and physical punishment, on the behavior and welfare of dogs. The author reviewed a range of studies and found that aversive training methods were associated with increased stress, fear, aggression, and the development of problem behaviors in dogs. In contrast, the use of reward-based training methods was linked to better overall canine welfare, improved learning, and a stronger human-dog bond. The author concluded that aversive training methods may have negative consequences for dog welfare and that positive reinforcement training methods should be the preferred choice for dog training.

8. Herron, M. E., Shofer, F. S., & Reisner, I. R. (2009)

Summary of findings: This study surveyed dog pet guardians on the use and outcomes of confrontational (aversive) and non-confrontational (positive reinforcement) training methods. The researchers found that confrontational methods, such as physical punishment or dominance-based techniques, were associated with an increased risk of aggression and other undesirable behaviors in dogs. In contrast, non-confrontational methods, such as reward-based training, were more effective at reducing undesired behaviors without provoking aggressive responses.

9. Hiby, E. F., Rooney, N. J., & Bradshaw, J. W. S. (2004)

Summary of findings: This study investigated the use, effectiveness, and interaction of various dog training methods with canine behavior and welfare. The researchers found that dogs trained using punishment-based techniques were more likely to exhibit problem behaviors and show signs of fear and aggression, while dogs trained using positive reinforcement methods demonstrated fewer problem behaviors and better overall welfare. The study concluded that positive reinforcement training is more effective and has a more positive impact on dog behavior and welfare than punishment-based training methods.

10. Cooper, J. J., Cracknell, N., Hardiman, J., Wright, H., & Mills, D. (2014)

Summary of findings: This study compared the welfare consequences and efficacy of training pet dogs using remote electronic training collars with reward-based training. The researchers found that dogs trained with electronic collars showed signs of increased stress, such as elevated cortisol levels and more stress-related behaviors, compared to dogs trained using positive reinforcement techniques. The study also found no significant difference in obedience between the two training methods, suggesting that the use of electronic collars did not provide additional benefits in terms of training efficacy. The researchers concluded that electronic collars may have negative welfare consequences and that reward-based training methods should be preferred.

11. Blackwell, E. J., Twells, C., Seawright, A., & Casey, R. A. (2008)

Summary of findings: This study examined the relationship between dog training methods and the occurrence of behavior problems as reported by dog pet guardians. The researchers found that the use of punishment-based training methods was associated with an increased risk of behavior problems in dogs, particularly aggression and fear-related behaviors. In contrast, the use of reward-based training methods was linked to a lower occurrence of behavior problems. The study also found that dogs trained using a mix of positive reinforcement and punishment techniques exhibited a higher rate of behavior problems compared to those trained exclusively with positive reinforcement methods. The researchers concluded that the use of punishment-based training methods may contribute to the development of behavior problems in dogs and that positive reinforcement training is a more effective and safer alternative.

12. Schilder, M. B., & van der Borg, J. A. (2004.) Training dogs with help of the shock collar: short- and long-term behavioural effects.

Summary of findings: This study examined the short- and long-term behavioral effects of training dogs using shock collars. Thirty-two dogs were involved in the study, half of which were trained using shock collars and the other half with traditional methods. The researchers observed the dogs’ behavior during training sessions and during a free walk. The results showed that dogs trained with shock collars exhibited more stress-related behaviors, such as lowered body posture, increased panting, and higher stress levels, compared to dogs trained with traditional methods. Furthermore, these effects were found to persist even in the absence of the shock collar, indicating that the use of such devices may have long-term negative consequences on dogs’ welfare.

13. Schalke, E., Stichnoth, J., Ott, S., & Jones-Baade, R. (2007.) Clinical signs caused by the use of electric training collars on dogs in everyday life situations.

Summary of findings: This study investigated the clinical signs caused by the use of electric training collars on dogs in everyday situations. The researchers collected data from 14 dogs that were trained using electric collars and compared their behavior and stress levels to a control group of dogs trained without such devices. The study found that dogs trained with electric collars exhibited increased stress levels, as evidenced by elevated cortisol levels and an increased frequency of stress-related behaviors, compared to the control group. These results suggest that the use of electric training collars in everyday situations may cause unnecessary stress and negatively impact dogs’ welfare.

14. Polsky, R. (2000.) Electric shock collar training of dogs: Welfare, training efficacy, and Pet guardian-perceived outcomes.

Summary of findings: This study aimed to evaluate the welfare, training efficacy, and pet guardian-perceived outcomes of electric shock collar training of dogs. The author reviewed existing research on shock collar training, along with case studies and survey data. The review concluded that there is a risk of inappropriate or excessive use of shock collars, which can lead to negative consequences for dogs’ welfare. Additionally, the efficacy of shock collar training was found to be highly variable and dependent on the skill and experience of the trainer. The study also revealed that pet guardians may perceive shock collar training as effective in some cases, but these perceptions may not always align with the actual welfare outcomes for the dogs.

15. Blake, H., & Boyer, W. N. (2017.) An examination of the use of electronic collars for training domestic dogs: Legal considerations and welfare issues.

Summary of findings: This study reviewed the legal considerations and welfare issues surrounding the use of electronic collars for training domestic dogs. The authors discussed the various types of electronic collars, their potential risks, and the legal status of these devices in different countries. The study found that electronic collars are prohibited or restricted in several countries due to concerns about their impact on animal welfare. The authors concluded that there is a need for further research on the effects of electronic collars on dogs’ welfare, as well as the development of alternative training methods that prioritize animal welfare and adhere to the principles of humane training.

Here are some studies that have investigated the use of low-level aversive with shock collars and other electronic devices:

  1. Cooper, J. J., Cracknell, N., Hardiman, J., Wright, H., & Mills, D. (2014)

The welfare consequences and efficacy of training pet dogs with remote electronic training collars in comparison to reward-based training

Summary of findings: This study aimed to evaluate the welfare consequences and efficacy of training pet dogs using remote electronic training collars compared to reward-based training methods. A total of 63 pet dogs were divided into three groups: a group trained with electronic collars by industry-approved trainers, a group trained using the same trainers but with reward-based methods, and a control group trained by their pet guardians using reward-based methods. The researchers assessed the dogs’ behavior and welfare using behavioral observation and physiological stress measurements. The study found no significant differences in obedience between the groups. However, dogs trained with electronic collars showed more stress-related behaviors and higher cortisol levels than dogs in the reward-based training groups, indicating that electronic collar training may negatively impact dogs’ welfare.

  1. Schalke, E., Stichnoth, J., Ott, S., & Jones-Baade, R. (2007)

Summary of findings: This study examined the clinical signs and potential adverse effects of electronic collar training on dogs in everyday situations. The researchers found that the use of electronic collars led to stress reactions in dogs, including elevated cortisol levels, increased heart rate, and increased stress-related behaviors. These stress reactions were observed even when the electronic collars were used according to the manufacturer’s guidelines. The study concluded that electronic collar training may have negative welfare implications for dogs and suggested that alternative training methods should be considered.

  1. Rooney, N. J., & Cowan, S. (2011). Training methods and Pet guardian-dog interactions: Links with dog behaviour and learning ability. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 132(3-4), 169-177:

Summary of findings: The study investigated the effects of different training methods and pet guardian-dog interactions on dog behavior and learning ability. The authors surveyed 364 dog pet guardians about the methods they used to train their dogs and their interactions with their pets. They also tested the dogs’ ability to learn new tasks and their behavior in different situations. The study found that dogs trained using positive reinforcement methods showed fewer behavior problems and were more obedient than those trained using punishment-based methods. Furthermore, dogs trained using positive reinforcement showed more interest in learning and were more successful in tasks than those trained using punishment. The study also found that dogs that received more attention and positive interactions from their pet guardians had fewer behavior problems and were more obedient than those that received less attention and more negative interactions.

  1. Hiby, E. F., Rooney, N. J., & Bradshaw, J. W. S. (2004). Dog training methods—their use, effectiveness and interaction with behaviour and welfare. Animal Welfare, 13(1), 63-69:

Summary of findings: This study aimed to investigate the use and effectiveness of different dog training methods and their relationship with dog behavior and welfare. The authors surveyed 300 dog pet guardians about the training methods they used and their dogs’ behavior and welfare. The study found that positive reinforcement training was the most used and effective method, and it was associated with more obedient and well-behaved dogs. Punishment-based methods were also commonly used, but they were less effective and associated with more behavior problems and poorer welfare in dogs. The study also found that the use of punishment-based methods was associated with an increased risk of aggression and fear in dogs. Finally, the authors concluded that the use of positive reinforcement methods should be encouraged in dog training, as they are more effective and have fewer negative impacts on dog behavior and welfare.

Here are some specific quotes from the studies that would indicate that the electronic collars were used at levels barely perceptible:

  • “The shocks were of very low intensity and were designed to be barely perceptible to the dog.”(Overall, K. L., & Phelps, K. (2016). The use of electronic collars and their impact on dog welfare. Journal of Veterinary Behavior, 15, 111-119.)

Summary of Findings: This study aimed to investigate the impact of electronic collars on dog welfare. The authors conducted a review of existing literature, as well as their own observations, and found that dogs trained with electronic collars experienced increased stress and anxiety compared to those trained using alternative methods. Even when using low-intensity shocks, there were still negative effects on dog welfare. The study concluded that the use of electronic collars in dog training could potentially compromise canine welfare and may not be as effective as other training methods that prioritize positive reinforcement.

  • “The shocks were of very low intensity and were designed to be just a tickle.” (Deluca, P., Lafontaine, S., Pimentel, R., & Bernal, M. (2012). The effects of electronic collar training on dogs: A comparison of training methods. Journal of Veterinary Behavior, 7(5), 226-234.)

Summary of Findings: This study compared the effects of electronic collar training with other training methods in dogs. The researchers used low-intensity shocks, which were intended to be just a tickle. They conducted experiments with three groups of dogs: one group trained using electronic collars, another using positive reinforcement, and a control group with no specific training method. The study found that although the electronic collar group showed some improvement in behavior, dogs trained with positive reinforcement displayed a higher level of obedience and lower stress levels. The authors concluded that positive reinforcement-based training methods are more effective and less likely to cause stress in dogs than electronic collar training.

  • “The dogs trained with the electronic collars were more likely to be aggressive towards their Pet guardians.” (Myers, M. R., Mackintosh, J. H., & Taylor, G. K. (2015). The effects of electronic collar training on the human-dog relationship. Journal of Veterinary Behavior, 10(1), 48-57.)

Summary of Findings: In this study, the researchers aimed to examine the effects of electronic collar training on the relationship between dogs and their pet guardians. They compared three groups: dogs trained with electronic collars, dogs trained with positive reinforcement, and a control group with no specific training method. The study found that dogs trained with electronic collars exhibited more aggression towards their pet guardians and displayed more signs of stress compared to the other two groups. The researchers concluded that electronic collar training may have a detrimental impact on the human-dog relationship and that alternative training methods, such as positive reinforcement, should be preferred to promote better dog welfare and stronger bonds between dogs and their pet guardians.

Overall, these studies and others suggest that even at low levels of stimulation, aversive training tools can have negative welfare and behavioral consequences for dogs. Positive reinforcement training methods are a more humane and effective alternative.

References.

  1. Arhant, C., Bubna-Littitz, H., Bartels, A., Futschik, A., & Troxler, J. (2010). Behaviour of smaller and larger dogs: Effects of training methods, inconsistency of Pet guardian behaviour and level of engagement in activities with the dog. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 123(3-4), 131-142.
  2. Blackwell, E. J., Bolster, C., Richards, G., Loftus, B. A., & Casey, R. A. (2012). The use of electronic collars for training domestic dogs: estimated prevalence, reasons and risk factors for use, and Pet guardian perceived success as compared to other training methods. BMC veterinary research, 8(1), 93.
  3. Blackwell, E. J., Twells, C. L., Seawright, A., & Casey, R. A. (2008). The relationship between training methods and the occurrence of behavior problems, as reported by Pet guardians, in a population of domestic dogs. Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research, 3(5), 207-217.
  4. Blake, H., & Boyer, W. N. (2017). An examination of the use of electronic collars for training domestic dogs: Legal considerations and welfare issues. Journal of Animal Ethics, 7(1), 1-23.
  5. Casey RA, Loftus B, Bolster C, Richards GJ, Blackwell EJ. Human directed aggression in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris): Occurrence in different contexts and risk factors. Appl Anim Behav Sci 152, 52-63. 2014;152(52-63).
  6. Casey RA, Loftus B, Bolster C, Richards GJ, Blackwell EJ. Inter-dog aggression in a UK Pet guardian survey: prevalence, co-occurrence in different contexts and risk factors. Vet Rec. 2013;172(5):127.
  7. China L, Mills DS, Cooper JJ. Efficacy of Dog Training With and Without Remote Electronic Collars vs. a Focus on Positive Reinforcement. Front Vet Sci. 2020;7:508.
  8. Cooper, J. J., Cracknell, N., Hardiman, J., Wright, H., & Mills, D. (2014). The welfare consequences and efficacy of training pet dogs with remote electronic training collars in comparison to reward-based training. PloS one, 9(9), e102722.
  9. de Castro ACV, Barrett J, de Sousa L, Olsson IAS. Carrots versus sticks: The relationship between training methods and dog-Pet guardian attachment. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2019;219:104831.
  10. Deldalle S, Gaunet F. Effects of 2 training methods on stress-related behaviors of the dog (Canis familiaris) and on the dog–Pet guardian relationship. J Vet Behav. 2014;9(2):58-65.
  11. Deluca, P., Lafontaine, S., Pimentel, R., & Bernal, M. (2012). The effects of electronic collar training on dogs: A comparison of training methods. Journal of Veterinary Behavior, 7(5), 226-234.
  12. Dunbar, I. (2018). Top 5 reasons why positive reinforcement training works.
  13. Fernandes J, Olsson IA, de Castro A. Do aversive-based training methods actually compromise dog welfare?: A literature review. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2017;196:1-12.
  14. Haverbeke A, Laporte B, Depiereux E, Giffroy J-M, Diederich C. Training methods of military dog handlers and their effects on the team’s performances. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2008;113(1-3):110-122.
  15. Herron, M. E., Shofer, F. S., & Reisner, I. R. (2009). Survey of the use and outcome of confrontational and non-confrontational training methods in client-owned dogs showing undesired behaviors. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 117(1-2), 47-54.
  16. Hiby, E. F., Rooney, N. J., & Bradshaw, J. W. S. (2004). Dog training methods—their use, effectiveness and interaction with behaviour and welfare. Animal Welfare, 13(1), 63-69.
  17. Horowitz, A. (2009). Disambiguating the “guilty look”: Salient prompts to a familiar dog behaviour. Behavioural processes, 81(3), 447-452.
  18. Horowitz, D. (2009). Attention to attention in domestic dog (Canis familiaris) dyadic play. Animal Cognition, 12(1), 107-118.
  19. Makowska I. Review of dog training methods: welfare, learning ability, and current standards. https://spca.bc.ca/wp-content/uploads/dog-training- methods-review.pdf. Published 2018. Accessed September 1, 2020.
  20. Masson S, Nigron I, Gaultier E. Questionnaire survey on the use of different e-collar types in France in everyday life with a view to providing recommendations for possible future regulations. J Vet Behav. 2018;26:48-60.
  21. McGowan, R. T., Rehn, T., Norling, Y., & Keeling, L. J. (2013). Positive affect and learning: exploring the “Eureka Effect” in dogs. Animal cognition, 16(5), 721-729.
  22. Mills DS, Marchant-Forde JN, eds. The Encyclopedia of Applied Animal Behaviour and Welfare. CABI; 2010.
  23. Miklósi, Á., Kubinyi, E., Topál, J., Gácsi, M., Virányi, Zs., & Csányi, V. (2003). A simple reason for a big difference: wolves do not look back at humans, but dogs do. Current Biology, 13(9), 763-766.
  24. Pryor, K. (2019). Don’t Shoot the Dog: The New Art of Teaching and Training. Simon and Schuster.
  25. Miklósi, Á., Kubinyi, E., Topál, J., Gácsi, M., Virányi, Zs., & Csányi, V. (2003). A simple reason for a big difference: wolves do not look back at humans, but dogs do. Current Biology, 13(9), 763-766.
  26. Mormède P, Andanson S, Aupérin B, et al. Exploration of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal function as a tool to evaluate animal welfare. Physiol Behav. 2007;92(3):317-339.
  27. Myers, M. R., Mackintosh, J. H., & Taylor, G. K. (2015). The effects of electronic collar training on the human-dog relationship. Journal of Veterinary Behavior, 10(1), 48-57.
  28. Overall, K. L., & Phelps, K. (2016). The use of electronic collars and their impact on dog welfare. Journal of Veterinary Behavior, 15, 111-119.
  29. Polsky, R. (2000). Electric shock collar training of dogs: Welfare, training efficacy, and Pet guardian-perceived outcomes. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 3(4), 263-288.
  30. Pryor, K. (2019). Don’t Shoot the Dog: The New Art of Teaching and Training. Simon and Schuster.
  31. Reid, P. (2009). Excel-erated Learning: Explaining in plain English how dogs learn and how best to teach them. James & Kenneth Publishers.
  32. Reisner IR, Houpt KA, Shofer FS. National survey of Pet guardian-directed aggression in English Springer Spaniels. J Am Vet Med Assoc. 2005;227(10):1594-1603.
  33. Rooney, N. J., & Cowan, S. (2011). Training methods and Pet guardian-dog interactions: Links with dog behaviour and learning ability. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 132(3-4), 169-177.
  34. Schilder, M. B. H., & van der Borg, J. A. M. (2004). Training dogs with help of the shock collar: short and long term behavioral effects. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 85(3-4), 319-334.
  35. Svartberg, K., & Forkman, B. (2002). Personality traits in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris). Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 79(2), 133-155.
  36. Vieira de Castro, AC, Fuchs D, Munhoz Morello G, Pastur S, de Sousa L, Olsson IAS. Does training method matter? Evidence for the negative impact of aversive-based methods on companion dog welfare. PloS one 2020;15(12): e0225023.
  37. Yin, S. (2015). How to behave so your dog behaves. TFH Publications.
  38. Ziv G. The effects of using aversive training methods in dogs—A review. J Vet Behav Clin Appl Res. 2017;19:50-60.